Childhood Syndrome

     _________________________________________________________________

  THE ETIOLOGY & TREATMENT OF CHILDHOOD

   Jordan W. Smoller, University of Pennsylvania

   Childhood is a syndrome which has only recently begun to receive
   serious attention from clinicians. The syndrome itself, however, is
   not at all recent. As early as the 8th century, the Persian historian
   Kidnom made references to "short, noisy creatures," who may well have
   been what we now call "children." The treatment of children, however,
   was unknown until this century, when so-called "child psychologists"
   and "child psychiatrists" became common. Despite this history of
   clinical neglect, it has been estimated that well over half of all
   Americans alive today have experienced childhood directly (Suess,
   1983). In fact, the actual numbers are probably much higher, since
   these data are based on self-reports which may be subject to social
   desirability biases and retrospective distortion.

   The growing acceptance of childhood as a distinct phenomenon is
   reflected in the proposed inclusion of the syndrome in the upcoming
   Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders, 4th edition, or
   DSM-IV, of the American Psychiatric Association (1990). Clinicians are
   still in disagreement about the significant clinical features of
   childhood, but the proposed DSM-IV will almost certainly include the
   following core features:
     * Congenital onset
     * Dwarfism
     * Emotional lability and immaturity
     * Knowledge deficits
     * Legume anorexia

   Clinical Features of Childhood:
   Although the focus of this paper is on the efficacy of conventional
   treatment of childhood, the five clinical markers mentioned above
   merit further discussion for those unfamiliar with this patient
   population.

      CONGENITAL ONSET

   In one of the few existing literature reviews on childhood, Temple-
   Black (1982) has noted that childhood is almost always present at
   birth, although it may go undetected for years or even remain
   subclinical indefinitely. This observation has led some investigators
   to speculate on a biological contribution to childhood. As one
   psychologist has put it, "we may soon be in a position to distinguish
   organic childhood from functional childhood" (Rogers, 1979).

      DWARFISM

   This is certainly the most familiar marker of childhood. It is widely
   known that children are physically short relative to the population at
   large. Indeed, common clinical wisdom suggests that the treatment of
   the so-called "small child" (or "tot") is particularly difficult.
   These children are known to exhibit infantile behavior and display a
   startling lack of insight (Tom and Jerry, 1967).

      EMOTIONAL LABILITY AND IMMATURITY

   This aspect of childhood is often the only basis for a clinician's
   diagnosis. As a result, many otherwise normal adults are misdiagnosed
   as children and must suffer the unnecessary social stigma of being
   labelled a "child" by professionals and friends alike.

      KNOWLEDGE DEFICITS

   While many children have IQ's with or even above the norm, almost all
   will manifest knowledge deficits. Anyone who has known a real child
   has experienced the frustration of trying to discuss any topic that
   requires some general knowledge. Children seem to have little
   knowledge about the world they live in. Politics, art, and science --
   children are largely ignorant of these. Perhaps it is because of this
   ignorance, but the sad fact is that most children have few friends who
   are not, themselves, children.

      LEGUME ANOREXIA

   This last identifying feature is perhaps the most unexpected. Folk
   wisdom is supported by empirical observation -- children will rarely
   eat their vegetables (see Popeye, 1957, for review).

    Causes of Childhood:

   Now that we know what it is, what can we say about the causes of
   childhood? Recent years have seen a flurry of theory and speculation
   from a number of perspectives. Some of the most prominent are reviewed
   below.

      Sociological Model

   Emile Durkind was perhaps the first to speculate about sociological
   causes of childhood. He points out two key observations about
   children:
   1) the vast majority of children are unemployed, and
   2) children represent one of the least educated segments of our
   society.

   In fact, it has been estimated that less than 20% of children have had
   more than fourth grade education.

   Clearly, children are an "out-group." Because of their intellectual
   handicap, children are even denied the right to vote. From the
   sociologist's perspective, treatment should be aimed at helping
   assimilate children into mainstream society. Unfortunately, some
   victims are so incapacitated by their childhood that they are simply
   not competent to work. One promising rehabilitation program (Spanky
   and Alfalfa, 1978) has trained victims of severe childhood to sell
   lemonade.

      Biological Model

   The observation that childhood is usually present from birth has led
   some to speculate on a biological contribution. An early investigation
   by Flintstone and Jetson (1939) indicated that childhood runs in
   families. Their survey of over 8,000 American families revealed that
   over half contained more than one child. Further investigation
   revealed that even most non-child family members had experienced
   childhood at some point. Cross-cultural studies (e.g., Mowgli & Din,
   1950) indicate that family childhood is even more prevalent in the Far
   East. For example, in Indian and Chinese families, as many as three
   out of four family members may have childhood.

   Impressive evidence of a genetic component of childhood comes from a
   large-scale twin study by Brady and Partridge (1972). These authors
   studied over 106 pairs of twins, looking at concordance rates for
   childhood. Among identical or monozygotic twins, concordance was
   unusually high (0.92), i.e., when one twin was diagnosed with
   childhood, the other twin was almost always a child as well.

      Psychological Models

   A considerable number of psychologically-based theories of the
   development of childhood exist. They are too numerous to review here.
   Among the more familiar models are Seligman's "learned childishness"
   model. According to this model, individuals who are treated like
   children eventually give up and become children. As a counterpoint to
   such theories, some experts have claimed that childhood does not
   really exist. Szasz (1980) has called "childhood" an expedient label.
   In seeking conformity, we handicap those whom we find unruly or too
   short to deal with by labelling them "children."

    Treatment of Childhood:

   Efforts to treat childhood are as old as the syndrome itself. Only in
   modern times, however, have humane and systematic treatment protocols
   been applied. In part, this increased attention to the problem may be
   due to the sheer number of individuals suffering from childhood.
   Government statistics (DHHS) reveal that there are more children alive
   today than at any time in our history. To paraphrase P.T. Barnum:
   "There's a child born every minute."

   The overwhelming number of children has made government intervention
   inevitable. The nineteenth century saw the institution of what remains
   the largest single program for the treatment of childhood -- so-called
   "public schools." Under this colossal program, individuals are placed
   into treatment groups based on the severity of their condition. For
   example, those most severely afflicted may be placed in a
   "kindergarten" program. Patients at this level are typically short,
   unruly, emotionally immature,and intellectually deficient. Given this
   type of individual, therapy is essentially one of patient management
   and of helping the child master basic skills (e.g. finger-painting).

   Unfortunately, the "school" system has been largely ineffective. Not
   only is the program a massive tax burden, but it has failed even to
   slow down the rising incidence of childhood.

   Faced with this failure and the growing epidemic of childhood, mental
   health professionals are devoting increasing attention to the
   treatment of childhood. Given a theoretical framework by Freud's
   landmark treatises on childhood, child psychiatrists and psychologists
   claimed great successes in their clinical interventions.

   By the 1950's, however, the clinicians' optimism had waned. Even after
   years of costly analysis, many victims remained children. The
   following case (taken from Gumbie & Poke, 1957) is typical.

     Billy J., age 8, was brought to treatment by his parents. Billy's
     affliction was painfully obvious. He stood only 4'3" high and
     weighed a scant 70 lbs., despite the fact that he ate voraciously.
     Billy presented a variety of troubling symptoms. His voice was
     noticeably high for a man. He displayed legume anorexia, and,
     according to his parents, often refused to bathe. His intellectual
     functioning was also below normal -- he had little general
     knowledge and could barely write a structured sentence. Social
     skills were also deficient. He often spoke inappropriately and
     exhibited "whining behaviour." His sexual experience was
     non-existent. Indeed, Billy considered women "icky." His parents
     reported that his condition had been present from birth, improving
     gradually after he was placed in a school at age 5. The diagnosis
     was "primary childhood." After years of painstaking treatment,
     Billy improved gradually. At age 11, his height and weight have
     increased, his social skills are broader, and he is now functional
     enough to hold down a "paper route."

   After years of this kind of frustration, startling new evidence has
   come to light which suggests that the prognosis in cases of childhood
   may not be all gloom. A critical review by Fudd (1972) noted that
   studies of the childhood syndrome tend to lack careful follow-up.
   Acting on this observation, Moe, Larrie, and Kirly (1974) began a
   large-scale longitudinal study. These investigators studied two
   groups. The first group consisted of 34 children currently engaged in
   a long-term conventional treatment program. The second was a group of
   42 children receiving no treatment. All subjects had been diagnosed as
   children at least 4 years previously, with a mean duration of
   childhood of 6.4 years.

   At the end of one year, the results confirmed the clinical wisdom that
   childhood is a refractory disorder -- virtually all symptoms persisted
   and the treatment group was only slightly better off than the
   controls.

   The results, however, of a careful 10-year follow-up were startling.
   The investigators (Moe, Larrie, Kirly , & Shemp, 1984) assessed the
   original cohort on a variety of measures. General knowledge and
   emotional maturity were assessed with standard measures. Height was
   assessed by the "metric system" (see Ruler, 1923), and legume appetite
   by the Vegetable Appetite Test (VAT) designed by Popeye (1968). Moe et
   al. found that subjects improved uniformly on all measures. Indeed, in
   most cases, the subjects appeared to be symptom-free. Moe et al.
   report a spontaneous remission rate of 95%, a finding which is certain
   to revolutionize the clinical approach to childhood.

   These recent results suggests that the prognosis for victims of
   childhood may not be so bad as we have feared. We must not, however,
   become too complacent. Despite its apparently high spontaneous
   remission rate, childhood remains one of the most serious and rapidly
   growing disorders facing mental health professional today. And, beyond
   the psychological pain it brings, childhood has recently been linked
   to a number of physical disorders. Twenty years ago, Howdi, Doodi, and
   Beauzeau (1965) demonstrated a six-fold increased risk of chicken pox,
   measles, and mumps among children as compared with normal controls.
   Later, Barby and Kenn (1971) linked childhood to an elevated risk of
   accidents -- compared with normal adults, victims of childhood were
   much more likely to scrape their knees, lose their teeth, and fall off
   their bikes. Clearly, much more research is needed before we can give
   any real hope to the millions of victims wracked by this insidious
   disorder.

      REFERENCES

     * American Psychiatric Association (1990). The diagnostic and
       statistical manual of mental disorders, 4th edition: A preliminary
       report. Washington, D.C.; APA.
     * Barby, B., & Kenn, K. (1971). The plasticity of behaviour. In B.
     * Barby & K. Kenn (Eds.), Psychotherapies R Us. Detroit: Ronco
       press.
     * Brady, C., & Partridge, S. (1972). My dads bigger than your dad.
       Acta Eur. Age, 9, 123-126.
     * Flintstone, F., & Jetson, G. (1939). Cognitive mediation of labour
       disputes. Industrial Psychology Today, 2, 23-35.
     * Fudd, E.J. (1972). Locus of control and shoe-size. Journal of
       Footwear Psychology, 78, 345-356.
     * Gumbie, G., & Pokey, P. (1957). A cognitive theory of
       iron-smelting. Journal of Abnormal Metallurgy, 45, 235-239.
     * Howdi, C., Doodi, C., & Beauzeau, C. (1965). Western civilization:
       A review of the literature. Reader's digest, 60, 23-25.
     * Moe, R., Larrie, T., & Kirly, Q. (1974). State childhood vs. trait
       childhood. TV guide, May 12-19, 1-3.
     * Moe, R., Larrie, T., Kirly, Q., & Shemp, C. (1984). Spontaneous
       remission of childhood In W.C. Fields (Ed.), New hope for children
       and animals. Hollywood: Acme Press.
     * Popeye, T.S.M. (1957). The use of spinach in extreme
       circumstances. Journal of Vegetable Science, 58, 530-538.
     * Popeye, T.S.M. (1968). Spinach: A phenomenological perspective.
       Existential botany, 35, 908-813.
     * Rogers, F. (1979). Becoming my neighbour. New York:Soft press.
     * Ruler, Y. (1923). Assessing measurements protocols by the
       multi-method multiple regression index for the psychometric
       analysis of factorial interaction. Annals of Boredom, 67,
       1190-1260.
     * Spanky, D., & Alfalfa, Q. (1978). Coping with puberty. Sears
       catalogue, 45-46.
     * Suess, D.R. (1983). A psychometric analysis of green eggs with and
       without ham. Journal of clinical cuisine, 245, 567-578.
     * Temple-Black, S. (1982). Childhood: an ever-so sad disorder.
       Journal of precocity, 3, 129-134.
     * Tom, C., & Jerry, M. (1967). Human behaviour as a model for
       understanding the rat. In M. de Sade (Ed.). The rewards of
       Punishment. Paris:Bench press.

      FURTHER READINGS

     * Christ, J.H. (1980). Grandiosity in children. Journal of applied
       theology, 1, 1-1000.
     * Joe, G.I. (1965). Aggressive fantasy as wish fulfilment. Archives
       of General MacArthur, 5, 23-45.
     * Leary, T. (1969). Pharmacotherapy for childhood. Annals of
       astrological Science, 67, 456-459.
     * Kissoff, K.G.B. (1975). Extinction of learnt behaviour. Paper
       presented to the Siberian Psychological Association, 38th annual
       Annual meeting, Kamchatka.
     * Smythe, C., & Barnes, T. (1979). Behaviour therapy prevents tooth
       decay. Journal of behavioral Orthodontics, 5, 79-89.
     * Potash, S., & Hoser, B. (1980). A failure to replicate the results
       of Smythe and Barnes. Journal of dental psychiatry, 34, 678-680.
     * Smythe, C., & Barnes, T. (1980). Your study was poorly done: A
       reply to Potash and Hoser. Annual review of Aquatic psychiatry,
       10, 123-156.
     * Potash, S., & Hoser, B. (1981). Your mother wears army boots: A
       further reply to Smythe and Barnes. Archives of invective
       research, 56, 5-9.
     * Smythe, C., & Barnes, T. (1982). Embarrassing moments in the sex
       lives of Potash and Hoser: A further reply. National Enquirer, May
       16.


------------------------------------------------------------------------
|  Michelle Steiner            | First say to yourself what you would  |
|  [email protected]            | be; and then do what you have to do.  |
| http://www.best.com/~steiner |     --Epictetus (55 - 135 CE)         |
------------------------------------------------------------------------
This page has been viewed by persons or bots.

Back to Home Page.

Hosted by www.Geocities.ws

1